Characteristics of memory associations: A consumer-based brand equity perspective

Krishnan, H

International Journal of Research in Marketing, Volume 13, Number 4, October 1996 , pp. 389-405(17)

1. Introduction

2. Background: Consumer associations

2.1. Number of associations
2.2..Valence of associations
2.3. Uniqueness of associations
2.4. Origin of associations

3. Study

3.1. Method
3.2. Measures
3.3. Results

4. Implications

4.1. Managerial issues: Understanding the association patterns
4.2. Measurement issues
4.3. Research issues: Expanding the model of memory associations

Abstract
This research uses a memory network model to identify various association characteristics underlying consumer-based brand equity. An empirical study measures association characteristics, such as set size, valence, uniqueness, and origin, and examines differences between high and low equity brands on these measures. The results show that consumer association differences are consistent with external equity indicators and provide insights on strong and weak areas for each brand that could be used to strengthen the brand. The discussion addresses implications of the association patterns for managing brand equity, compares the association measures with other equity measurement approaches, and broadens the set of association concepts used in this research.

1. Introduction

2. Background: Consumer associations

2.1. Number of associations

2.2..Valence of associations

2.3. Uniqueness of associations

2.4. Origin of associations

3. Study

3.1. Method

3.2. Measures

3.3. Results

4. Implications

4.1. Managerial issues: Understanding the association patterns

4.2. Measurement issues

4.3. Research issues: Expanding the model of memory associations


브랜드에 대한 외부 수치에 의해 각 카테고리별로 High/Low 브랜드를 선정
연상네트워크상의 지표들의 우위를 비교해서 실제 외적 수치와 얼마나 일치하는지를 평가함.

15년전 이야기...
블로그 이미지

브라보맨

,

Advances in Consumer Research Volume19, 1992      Pages 782-786

A SPREADING ACTIVATION MODEL OF CONSUMERS' ASYMMETRIC SIMILARITY JUDGMENT

Ehsan Ulhaque, University of Texas at Arlington

Kenneth D. Bahn, University of Texas at Arlington

ABSTRACT -

Consumers often make asymmetric similarity judgments which are explainedvia Tversky's contrast model. This paper attempts to explain asymmetricsimilarity judgments by using the spreading activation models of semantic anddeclarative memory. It is proposed that the time taken for activation to spreadfrom one node to the next is related to, and gives rise to, asymmetricsimilarity judgment.

의미 기억과 기술적 기억의 활성화 확산 모델을 이용한 비대칭적 유사성 판단 설명.


INTRODUCTION

An essential component in the study of marketing and consumer behavior is that of consumer judgment and choice processes. Theses judgment and choice processes are based, to some extent, on cognitive representations -- consumers' internal memory structures -- as well as information which is acquired externally (Bettman 1979; Johnson and Puto 1987). While a plethora of research had been conducted on what, how and why of external information acquisition, research on cognitive representations of consumers has been rather limited (Gardner, Mitchell and Russo 1978; Johnson and Puto 1987). This state of affairs is rather sad as cognitive psychology provides a number of research streams and models in the area of cognitive representation (Anderson 1983; collins and Loftus 1975; Smith, Shoben and Rips 1974). This paper provides ans indirect testing of cognitive representation of brands in the tradition of Gardener, Mitchell and Russo (1978) and Meyers-Levy (1989).

....

Specifically, a network model of declarative memory (Anderson 1983) is proposed. The nodes of this network are occupied by brands with their related network of attributes. When nodes are activated, activation spreads to all other nodes connected to the activated node (collins and Loftus 1975). The time taken for activation to spread from one node to another is hypothesized to be linked to similarity judgment. Asymmetric similarity judgment is proposed to be the result of differential time taken for the activation to spread from one node to another.

MODELS OF SEMANTIC MEMORY

Although various models of semantic memory have been proposed (e.g., Hierarchical Network model of Collins and Quillian 1969; Feature-Comparison model of Smith, Shoben Rips 1974; Spreading-Activation model 0f Collins and Loftus 1975) we will focus on spreading activation models. The reason being that the spreading activation, or network, models have better fared th scholarly scrutiny of various researchers (Chang 1986)

According to the spreading activation model of Collins and Loftus (976), the concepts (or brands in this case) are represented in memory as nodes, and relations between brands are represented as associated pathways between the nodes. There could be multiple paths of various strengths and lengths between brands. 두 node가 공유하는 속성 node가 많을 수록 두 node 간의 관계는 더 가까워지고 커진다. 두 활성화 확산이 교차하는 지점에서 의사 결정이 일어 난다(Collins and Loftus 1975).

Anderson (1983) has proposed a similar process in his ACT model of human memory. According to his model, brands or nodes have an activation value associated with them which reflect the current importance, relevance, or availability of the node in memory. the links between nodes have weights associated with them which are determined by the strength of associations between the connected nodes. For example, if coke is regarded as the most popular soft drink then the link associating node 'Coke' to node 'soft drink' will have more weight associated with it reflecting the strength the strength of association between the two nodes. On the other hand, if coke is not thought of as a food item then the weight of the link associating node 'Coke' to node 'food' will be relatively low, or there will be no such link. When a node is activated the spread of activation is modulated by the weights associated with each link, i.e., closely associated nodes produce higher activation transfer as compared to weakly associated nodes. Reder and Anderson (1980) found that the amount of activation spreading from a given node along a pathway is a function of the strength of that pathway relative to the sum of strengths of all paths emanating from that node.

Priming tasks have provided sufficient empirical support to network models of memory (Ashcraft 1989). In priming tasks a prime stimulus, which is presented first, influences some later process on a target stimulus which follows the prime. '닥터'로 점화 될 때 보다 '빵'으로 점화 될 때 '버터'에 대한 판단 속도가 더 빨랐다(Meyer and Schvaneveldt 1971). 빵은 버터와 관련이 있는 개념이기 때문에 빵으로 점화 되면 버터가 이미 활성화된 상태이기 때문에 닥터로 점화 되었을 때 보다 버터에 대한 반응이 더 빠르다.

Anderson의 모델에서 가장 중요한 것은 'Fan Effect' 이다. 가지고 있는 연결의 수가 많으면 활성화 확산되는 크기가 작아지는 효과. Meyers-Levy (1989a)에서 밝혀짐.

The concept of spreading activation has been used to explain mechanisms involved in such tasks as category exemplar production (Loftus 1973), semantic priming in lexical decisions (Neely 1977), episodic sentence and word recognition (Anderson 1983), and reading comprehension (Stanovich and West 1983).


CONTRAST MODEL OF SIMILARITY JUDGMENT

A SPREADING ACTIVATION MODEL OF ASYMMETRIC SIMILARITY JUDGMENT

CONCLUSIONS

블로그 이미지

브라보맨

,

The Influence of a Brand Name's Association Set Size and Word Frequency on Brand Memory

   

Joan Meyers-Levy

The Journal of Consumer Research

Vol. 16, No. 2 (Sep., 1989) (pp. 197-207)

원문보기

   

Brand names serve a variety of purposes for both consumers and advertisers. Perhaps their simplest function is to provide labels by which firms identify and promote their products and services to consumers (Friedman 1985). However, because brand names represent the rich configurations of symbols and meaning that are embodied by products (Levy 1979), they can also take on their own meaning and presence. Through their linguistic characteristics or associations, brand names can influence the product inferences and evaluations consumers form (Peterson and ross 1972). Moreover, firms with extremely memorable brand names (e.g., Coca0Cola, McDonalds) often regard the names as their most valuable asset because these labels provide immediate recognition and, often, acceptance of new products that maybe introduced under the brand name. Thus, a critical question is, what dimensions of a brand name make it memorable?

One intuitively reasonable thesis is that band name memorability might be enhanced when the name can be meaningfully related to many other concepts already stored in memory. In this article, the term association set refers to such groups of concepts that are meaningfully related to a target word or brand name. The logic underlying this view is that the abundant concepts associated with the brans name word provide rich networks of pathways for retrieving the name.

 

  

블로그 이미지

브라보맨

,
Construal Levels and Psychological Distance:
Effects on Representation, Prediction, Evaluation, and Behavior

Y Trope, N Liberman, C Wakslak

Journal of Consumer Psychology (2007)
Volume: 17, Issue: 2, Publisher: Lawrence Erlbaum, Pages: 83-95




LEVEL OF CONSTRUAL

DISTANCE AND MENTAL REPRESENTATION
Distance and Mental Representation: Time
Distance and Mental Representation: Space
Distance and Mental Representation: Social Distance
Distance and Mental Representation: Probability
Pictorial vs. Verbal Representation
Abstraction and Distance Judgments

DISTANCE AND PREDICTION

DISTANCE AND EVALUATION
Primary vs. Secondary Features
Desirability vs. Feasibility
Pros vs. Cons
Idealistic Values vs. Pragmatic Concerns
Alignable vs. Nonalignable Attributes

DISTANCE AND BEHAVIOR
Predicting Behavioral Intentions from Attitudes and Values
Logrolling in Negotiation
Self-control
Sunk-cost Bias
Risk Perceptions and Temporal Frames
Consumer Impatience

TOWARD A THEORY OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISTANCE

CONCLUSIONS


해석 수준 이론은 심리적 거리가 개인의 생각과 행동에 어떻게 영향을 미치는지를 설명하는 이론임
CLT의 기본적인 가정
- 심리적으로 가까운 대상 -> 낮은 수준, 구체적, 맥락화된 특성
- 심리적으로 먼 대상 -> 높은 수준, 추상적, 안정적인 특성

LEVEL OF CONSTRUAL
CLT에 따르면, 사람들은
가까운 이벤트를 표상하기 위해 -> 구체적이고 낮은 수준의 해석을 하고
먼 이벤트를 표상하기 위해 -> 추상적이고 높은 수준의 해석을 한다.

추상화의 과정이 실무율적이지는 않음 -> 높은 위계로 올라갈 수록, 표상은 점점 덜 구체적이고 더 추상적이게 됨
어떤 일이 심리적으로 더 멀어질 수록, 더 높은 수준으로 추상적으로 표상됨.

심리적 거리와 추상성간의 가정된 관계는 직접적인 경험과 사건에 대한 정보간에 존재하는 연합의 결과이다.
- 직접적인 경험을 할 때 -> 가용할 수 있는 정보가 많으므로 구체적인 것들이 떠오르지만
- 직접적인 경험이 없을 때 -> 가용할 수 있는 정보가 적어지므로 추상적인 것들이 떠오르게 된다.


DISTANCE AND MENTAL REPRESENTATION
Distance and Mental Representation: Time
먼 미래의 일은 가까운 미래의 것 보다 더 추상적이고, 구조화되며, 높은 수준으로 표상됨

Liberman, Sagristano, and Trope (2002)

Wakslak, Nussbaum, Liberman, and Trope (2006) 

Liberman andTrope (1998)
-

Day and Bartels (2004)
-

Nussbaum, Trope, and Liberman (2003)
-

Förster, Friedman, and Liberman(2004)


-> 먼 미래의 사건은 추상적으로 표상되며, 상의의 특성을 상징하는 상태로 구조화 됨
-> 가까운 미래의 사건은 구체적으로 표상되고, 하위의 특성에 대한 상징을 포함하는 상태로 구조화 됨

Distance and Mental Representation: Space

Fujita,Henderson, Eng, Trope, & Liberman, 2006
- 필름 속의 사람들이 먼 곳에 있다고 느낀 사람들은 가깝게 있다고 느낀 사람들에 비해,
  필름 속의 사건을 더 추상적인 언어로 표현했음.


Distance and Mental Representation: Social Distance

Liviatan, Trope, and Liberman (2006)
- 비슷하지 않은 대상의 행동이, 비슷한 대상의 행동 보다 더 높은 수준의 용어로 표상됨

Smith and Trope(2006)
- 큰 힘으로 점화된 피험자들은 더 추상적인 생각을 했음 -> 더 넓은 폭의 카테고리화를 했음.


Distance and Mental Representation: Probability

Wakslak, Trope, Liberman, and Alony (2006)
  

Pictorial vs. Verbal Representation

Amit (2006)
Abstraction and Distance Judgments

DISTANCE AND PREDICTION

DISTANCE AND EVALUATION
Primary vs. Secondary Features
Desirability vs. Feasibility
Pros vs. Cons
Idealistic Values vs. Pragmatic Concerns
Alignable vs. Nonalignable Attributes

DISTANCE AND BEHAVIOR
Predicting Behavioral Intentions from Attitudes and Values
Logrolling in Negotiation
Self-control
Sunk-cost Bias
Risk Perceptions and Temporal Frames
Consumer Impatience

TOWARD A THEORY OF PSYCHOLOGICAL DISTANCE

CONCLUSIONS
블로그 이미지

브라보맨

,
One brand perception? Or many? The heterogeneity of intra-brand knowledge

Oliver Koll, Sylvia von Wallpach



  1. Introduction
  2. Brand strength: the brand knowledge-response link
  3. Brand response segments
  4. Method
  5. Results
  6. Discussion
  7. Limitations and future research

블로그 이미지

브라보맨

,